In
a series of posts this week, I will discuss ongoing work that I
and several other scholars are doing on cultural cognition. The
posts will describe the basic theory of cultural cognition, the methods we are
using to investigate it, our key empirical findings relating to it, and some of
its implications (positive and normative) for law.
But
to introduce the topic, I thought it might be interesting to describe one
particular, and particularly provocative, dynamic associated with cultural
cognition. I’ll call it the “white male status-anxiety effect.”
This
label (which I’ll admit is not 100% accurate as a descriptive matter!) is a
play on the “white male effect,” a term used by scholars of risk perception.
See, e.g., Melissa Finucane, Paul Slovic, C.K. Mertz, James Flynn & Theresa
A. Satterfield, Gender, Race, and Perceived Risk: The "White Male"
Effect, 3 Health, Risk, & Soc'y 159 (2000). White males, these
scholars have shown, are less concerned with all manner of risk--of global warming, gun accidents, various medical procedures,
etc.--than are women and minorities. Various hypotheses have been
offered to explain the white male effect: that white men
are more informed than women and minorities, that women and minorities feel
more vulnerable or less able to protect themselves, that women (and perhaps
minorities) are more empathetic than white men. But none has been borne
out by empirical examination.
So we (Paul Slovic, John Gastil, Don Braman, C.K. Mertz and I) decided to consider another possibility. The reason white males are less afraid of various risks, we surmised, is that they (or really, certain ones of them) are more afraid of something else, viz., the loss of status they would experience were activities essential to their cultural roles stigmatized as dangerous and worthy of regulation.
This
hypothesis is grounded in what we call the cultural cognition thesis. Founded
on an amalgam of sources in the social sciences (about which I’ll say more in
later posts!), the cultural cognition thesis asserts that individuals tend to
conform their beliefs about the risks posed by putatively dangerous activities
to their culturally grounded moral appraisals of those activities. If an
activity is status-enhancing for individuals of a particular cultural
persuasion, such individuals are motivated by a form of psychological
self-defense mechanism (we call it “status anxiety”) to resist the claim that
the activity is dangerous and should be regulated. By the same token, if the
activity is one that is status-diminishing within a cultural group, members of
that group are motivated to embrace those same claims.
The
influence of cultural cognition can be expected to generate differences in risk
perception across individuals of varying cultural perspectives. But it also, we
hypothesized, ought to create certain differences within groups of persons who
share a cultural outlook. The activities (carrying a gun, engaging in commerce,
raising a child) that confer status within a particular cultural group are
likely to be gender- and race-specific. Accordingly, we should
expect to see differences in risk perception that correspond to the diverse
values that persons of varying combinations of demographic characteristics and
cultural outlooks place on those activities.
In
a national
study of risk and culture (more on this, too, in a future post), we
confirmed this general hypothesis as it applied to a series of putatively
dangerous activities. One of these was owning a gun. Guns bear positive moral
connotations for persons of hierarchic and individualistic worldviews.
Consistent with the cultural cognition thesis, we found that individuals who
hold such worldviews are much less likely to believe that guns are
dangerous--either for their owners or for society--than are individuals who
hold egalitarian and solidaristic views (again, I’ll have more to say about
these “worldview” categories--their derivation and our measures of them--in
future posts).
But
even more important, risk skepticism about guns was most pronounced among white
male hierarchists and male individualists. We had predicted this
pattern on the ground that the individualist virtues associated with
guns--courage, physical prowess, self-reliance--and the hierarchic roles enabled
by them--father, hunter, provider--are primarily status-enchaining for men
within those groups. Male individualists and hierarchists thus have a much
larger psychic investment in resisting the claim that guns are dangerous and
worthy of regulation than does anyone else. Moreover, because the hierarchic
associations that guns bear have historically been confined to whites, white
male hierarchs have the biggest investment of all in seeing guns as safe
(indeed, we found, that such individuals believe that gun ownership enhances
rather than reduces public safety).
The gender- and race-specificity of cultural differences, we found, totally explained the “white male effect” with respect to gun risks. That is, all the differences between men and women and between whites and blacks observed in our sample generally (N=1800) were attributable to the extreme differences between male and female individualists, and between white male hierarchs and all other hierarchs. There is no “white male effect” per se, but rather a “male individualist” and a “white male hierarch” effect--which fits our hypothesis that these individuals experience the greatest status anxiety when guns are asserted to be dangerous and worthy of regulation.
We
found a similar relationship between the cultural status anxiety and the white
male effect in environmental risk perceptions. To begin with, there are no
differences in risk perception across race once cultural worldviews are
controlled for. Gender differences do persist. But they are due entirely
to the wide discrepancy in the views of extremely risk-skeptical white
hierarchical males and considerably less risk-skeptical hierarchical women.
There are no gender (or race) based differences in environmental risk
perception among relatively individualistic or egalitarian persons.
Again,
these patterns suggest the impact of culture-specific gender differences in
status-conferring social roles. Within a hierarchic way of life, men tend to
earn esteem by achieving success in civil society, while women earn it by
successfully occupying domestic roles. Accordingly, it is hierarchic men, not
hierarchic women, who experience the greatest status threat when commercial and
industrial activities are challenged as dangerous. But within an individualist
way of life, success in the market is status-conferring for men and
women. Accordingly, individualistic men and individualistic women react
with status-protecting skepticism when commerce and industry are attacked as
dangerous. Commerce and industry are symbolic of social inequality and
unconstrained individualism within egalitarian and solidaristic ways of life.
Accordingly, as a means of promoting their status, men and women alike within
these cultural groups tend to embrace claims of environmental risk.
It
would be wrong to suggest that white hierarchical or individualistic men are
the only ones whose risk perceptions are shaped by status anxieties. Indeed, we
found that status concerns also help to explain interesting variations in risk
perception among women relating to the dangers of obtaining an abortion.
Hierarchical women but not individualistic or egalitarian ones perceive
obtaining an abortion to be very dangerous to a woman’s health. Sociologist Kristin Luker depicts
abortion as the symbolic focal point in a status conflict between two groups of
women: those who subscribe to hierarchical norms that confer esteem upon women
who occupy domestic roles such as motherhood; and those who adhere to individualistic
and egalitarian norms that confer esteem upon women and men alike for
successfully occupying professional roles. It is thus status protective for the
former group of women to accept the asserted health risks of abortion and for
the latter to reject these asserted risks.
What
are the normative and prescriptive upshots of these findings for law? I think
there are some, and they are important. But I’ve said more than enough and will
leave this as one point readers might like to pursue through discussion.
I
will, however, conclude with one more specific point about method. Previous
investigators of the “white male effect” (including some who collaborated in
this study) had failed to explain it fully not just because they had an
incomplete set of explanatory variables but also because they posited too
simple a relationship between them and risk perception. In effect, those
investigators had assumed that gender and race must be proxies for some other
sort of individual difference--in empathy, in political vulnerability, in
education, etc.; to test this hypothesis, they devised measures of these others
differences and examined whether including them as independent variables
eliminated the explanatory power of gender and race in multivariate regression analyses.
The
relatively simple but important innovation in our model was to examine how race
and gender interact with our key variables of interest. That is, we did
not simply plug cultural orientation into our regression as a separate
independent variable and see if it neutralized the effect of race and gender. Instead,
because our hypothesis was that race and gender effects are culture-specific,
we created interaction variables that allowed us to examine how being a man or
a woman, or a white or a black influences an individual's risk perceptions as that individual’s
cultural orientation is made to vary (and all other influences are held
constant). We found that these variables were significant and had the predicted
signs. And we were able to determine (through separate analyses in which dummy
variables were used to remove these effects) that the observed interactions
accounted for the “white male” effect with respect to the risks we
investigated.
The analyses appear in a draft paper that reports our results. But you can also check out some neat figures that graphically illustrate the interaction effects I’ve described.
Male Status is only men to increase penile size
Posted by: vimax | 13 April 2010 at 10:02 PM
Nice story this is really interesting
Posted by: vimax | 03 June 2009 at 04:07 AM
Hi, Jeff. This is a good point. I can tell you that demographic variables and other individual characteristics on which we collected information explained only a modest amount of the variation -- 11%-- in cultural worldviews in our sample. But I'm guessing structural equation modeling could usefully explore the issue whether a model that posits demographic variables > culture > risk perception fits better than one that treats culture and demographic variables as independent infuences (although ones that do interact in some cases) on risk perception. John Gastil, another of our team members, has more experience w/ structural equation modeling; I'll ask him what he thinks. Thanks again!
Posted by: Dan Kahan | 07 June 2006 at 08:42 AM
Dan, thanks for your wonderful post. I think that the basic theory that you posit is both cogent and very useful; I was primarily interested in the origins of culture type as a possible extension of the theory. One aspect of origins that might be of interest regarding your theory is the idea that demographic factors and environment might have both direct effects (which you demonstrate are less powerful explanations than culture type) *and* indirect effects in that they may influence culture type.
********************
Jeff Yates
Associate Professor
Department of Political Science
University of Georgia
http://www.uga.edu/pol-sci/people/yates.htm
SSRN: http://ssrn.com/author=454290
*********************
Posted by: Jeff Yates | 07 June 2006 at 08:11 AM
Hi, Jeff. I discuss the culture categories, which we adapted from Mary Douglas, a bit more in today's post. But I admit to be embarrassingly perplexed about the origins of the categories. And at 2 levels, as it were: (a) societal: why are these remarkably spare categories the right ones? and (b) individual: how do individuals acquire their orientations?
The most systematic attempt to work out Culture Theory of the Douglas sort is Michael Thompson, Richard Ellis & Aaron Wildavsky, Cultural Theory (1990), but it fails, in my view, to offer very good answers to either of these questions. They take a functional view, arguing that the culture types are necessary for society's to flourish, and that individuals essentially take on the worldviews in question *b/c* that conduces to the flourishing of the types.... I don't buy it!
I lean toward these answers:
(a) Douglas' group-grid scheme is just a very parsimonious way to capture what are likely to be the most important distinctions in modes of social organization. Indeed, the 2X2 that she uses (see more in my post from today) is shared by many many shemes in the social sciences. They are gross simplifications-- but they are still useful for explaining, testing, and possibly policy design.
(b) Individuals likely acquire the cultural orientations through socialization, although it is possible, I suppose, that there is some genetic contribution to the group-grid outlooks. But I think it's fair to take individuals' orientations as exogenous methodologically, given that they are reliable and explantory (when measured with our cultural worldview scales), and morally, given how problematic it would be for government to make shaping orientations an object of policy.
But if you think the origins of the culture type does important implications for the cogency or usefulness of the basic theory, I'm all ears!
Posted by: Dan Kahan | 06 June 2006 at 10:26 AM
Dan, thanks for your thoughtful answer to my question. Given that the cultural worldview (e.g. egalitarian vs. hierarchy) seems to be the touchstone modifier variable, I suppose the follow up question would be - what explains the egalitarian/hierarchy distinction?
I assume that this measure is derived from survey responses, but if it is the most powerful explanation of risk behavior, then it seems appropriate to ask - what are its determinants? Certainly this might be a compelling extension of the original question if not already answered. Is egal/hierarchy an inate characteristic, or is it primarily environmentally driven? If the latter, then what environmental factors influence a person to be at one end or the other of this cultural worldview spectrum?
Just some thoughts on this intriguing topic.
BTW, Woody Allen called and said that he wouldnt want to be part of any survey that would have him as a respondent ;-)
********************
Jeff Yates
Associate Professor
Department of Political Science
University of Georgia
http://www.uga.edu/pol-sci/people/yates.htm
SSRN page: http://ssrn.com/author=454290
*********************
Posted by: Jeff Yates | 05 June 2006 at 06:23 PM
Hi, Jeff. One of the takeaways of this particular application of cultural cognition is that it is indeed misleading to aggregate all white males when trying to explain variation in risk perception. Being a white male, we found, matters only if (and to the extent) one holds hierarchical and individualist worldviews.
Indeed, the "white male effect" has always been understood as a puzzle waiting to be solved. No one really thought that that demographic characteristic could be causing variation in risk perception; everyone thought it correlated with some other factor that did. In fact, the solution to the puzzle is even more complicated, since it isn't the *correlation* between white maleness and anything else that explains the "white male effect"; rather, it's the *interaction* of white maleness with cultural worldviews that does.
As for other individual characteristics: we found that the influence of cultural worldviews on gun risks was much larger than any other factor, including where one lives (rural vs. urban, south vs. north, etc.), income, political ideology, personality type, gender, race, etc. You can find the data (and download a mean-ass regression table) here: http://research.yale.edu/culturalcognition/index.php?option=content&task=view&id=98
The same was true about the relative impact of culture and other influences on various other types of risk perceptions.
I'm not sure where Woody Allen would fit in our research, but if you have his email, I'll send him a written version of our survey!
Posted by: Dan Kahan | 05 June 2006 at 01:53 PM
Dan,
I find this line of research very interesting. I'm wondering though, if there may be factors that need to be considered that are not addressed in your post. I haven't read all of the underlying work, so there may be an easy answer to my question, but here goes:
Is it the case that all white males are appropriately aggregated? (Or African American males, for that matter); Is it possible that there may be additional considerations at work here?
With regard to the gun control issue, might it be the case that other demographic distinctions, such as Rural vs. Urban, or Socio-economic status, yield important insights concerning this general theory?
I could be wrong, but I just don't see Woody Allen (very urban and high s/e status) getting all worked up over somebody regulating his collection of guns.
On the other hand, he did make a pretty convincing gun-toting dictator in "Bananas". ;-)
********************
Jeff Yates
Associate Professor
Department of Political Science
University of Georgia
http://www.uga.edu/pol-sci/people/yates.htm
SSRN page: http://ssrn.com/author=454290
*********************
Posted by: Jeff Yates | 05 June 2006 at 01:22 PM
I agree that:
a. cultural cognition is driving persons on *both sides* of the gun debate and various other risk-regulation issues;
b. there is nothing distinctively "white male" about it (indeed, our results show there's no "white male effect" per se in risk perception);
c. that one side or the other is likely "right" as an empirical matter; and
d. that it's unlikely that any of the cultural groups is right about everything.
I *almost* agree with your point that "it come[s] down to who is more correct." I agree with the basic thrust of your point that it doesn't discredit a position to show that those who hold it do so as a result of culturally grounded status anxiety. But I don't accept, necessarily, that what's normative here is who is "correct" on the empirics in the various debates where cultural cognition is in play. Part of what's going on is that individuals are perceiving that empirical claims that fit withing a utilitiarn or economic calculus support support states of affairs that they value on non-utilitiarian, cultural grounds. They might be *wrong* about the empirics but still right, morally speaking, to prefer the state of affairs that they value for cultural reasons.... More on this in a forthcoming post on the prescriptive/normative implications of cultural cognition!
Posted by: Dan Kahan | 05 June 2006 at 01:13 PM
I understand and your theory is surely a plausible one. But I suspect you could come up with a comparably plausible theory to explain the egalitarians. I suspect both have truth.
Ultimately, doesn't it come down to who is more correct? E.g., if gun control is a good idea that will save lives, you might criticize the white male perspective, but if it is a bad idea, not.
I can think of a lot of environmental issues where the white male perception was the more correct one (though certainly not all environmental issues). This doesn't refute the possibility that the position is attributable to status anxiety but it suggests to me that the contrary position is also driven by psychological characteristics rather than a logical assessment. I bet the underlying syndrome is more of a "human" effect than a "white male" effect.
Posted by: frank cross | 05 June 2006 at 12:43 PM
Good question. My first instinct is to concede the point completely. Our theory is that individuals of all cultural orientations form risk perceptions (and related beliefs) in a manner that avoids the sort of psychological cost associated with being at odds with other members of their in-group and with having to believe that the others within their in-group are ignorant or evil. So the disposition of egalitarians and solidarists to perceive guns, say, as risky is just as much a status-anxiety effect as the disposition of hierarchs and individualists to see them as safe. (Of course, one group still might be right and the other wrong; correctness of the view isn't the baseline, that's for certain.)
But the only reason I won't happily concede everything is that the "white male" findings have to do w/ interesting gender (and race) differences within groups of persons who share a cultural worldview. You might talk me out of it--indeed, I almost hope you do--but I feel it is more natural (in the sanse, of more useful for explantory purposes) to assign greater or lesser amounts of "status anxiety" to one or the other to explain that. On guns, for example, hierarchical and individualist worldviews predict the view that guns are "safe" among both men *and* women; however, the effect of those worldviews on men is much much larger. That fits our theory insofar as we say hierarchical and individualist men have a bigger investment in access to guns to perform roles that are status-enhancing for them. But insofar as inividualist and hierarchist women still are more disposed to see guns as "safe" than are solidarist and egalitarian ones, it seems strange to say they are holding back on status-anxiety grounds; female hierarchs and individualists are motivated by status-anxiety relative to egalitarians and solidarists, but not nearly *as much* as male hierarchs and individualists. Or at least I find myself drawn to that way of talking for explanatory purposes.
Posted by: Dan Kahan | 05 June 2006 at 10:51 AM
Interesting findings, but I wonder about your benchmark. While we have differences among groups, how do you know it is the white males, or hierarchists, who have the status anxieties, as opposed to other groups?
Posted by: frank cross | 05 June 2006 at 09:35 AM